Communications in Kinesiology
Limited effects of age on the use of the ankle and counter-rotation mechanism in the sagittal plane during standing
Authors: Maud van den Bogaart, Sjoerd M. Bruijn, Joke Spildooren, Jaap H. van Dieën, Pieter Meyns
Editor: Matthieu Boisgontier
DOI: 10.51224/cik.2023.46
Last Updated: May 9, 2023
Abstract
During standing, the center of mass (CoM) can be accelerated to remain within the base of support by applying ankle moments to shift the center of pressure (CoP mechanism). An additional mechanism is the counter-rotation mechanism, i.e., changing the angular momentum of segments around the CoM to change the direction of the ground reaction force. In this study, we assessed anteroposterior balance performance and the related use of these postural control mechanisms in children, younger and older adults. Sixteen children (6-9y), 17 younger adults (18-24y) and eight older adults (65-80y) performed bipedal upright standing trials of 16 seconds on a rigid surface and on three balance boards, varying in height (15-19 cm), that could freely move in the sagittal plane,. Full body kinematics were retrieved via a Simi 3D motion analysis system (GmbH), DeepLabCut and Anipose. Performance related outcome measures, i.e., the number of trials with balance loss, standard deviation of the time series of the CoM acceleration (due to the CoP and counter-rotation mechanism) and the contributions of the CoP and counter-rotation mechanism to the CoM acceleration (in %) were calculated. Furthermore, selected kinematic measures, i.e., the orientation of the board and head and the Mean Power Frequency of board orientation and of CoM acceleration were calculated. Compared to younger adults, children and older adults showed a poorer balance performance. Across age groups and conditions, the contribution of the CoP mechanism to the total CoM acceleration was dominant, i.e., 95%-108%. The contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism was limited, i.e., 19%-31% (with totals higher than 100% indicating opposite effects of both mechanisms), which could be due to the fact that the counter-rotation mechanism would conflict with stabilizing the head in space. Furthermore, children used the counter-rotation mechanism relatively more compared to younger adults. This could indicate that they are still learning to limit the contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism.
1 Introduction
Adequate postural control is a prerequisite for performance of crucial activities of daily life. Postural control is necessary to prevent falls and can be defined as controlling the state of the body center of mass (CoM, i.e., the point around which the mass is evenly distributed) relative to the base of support (BoS, i.e., the area within an outline of all points on the body which are directly in contact with the support surface) (Horak, 1987). Postural control is regulated by the sensorimotor control system; this integrates sensory input from visual, vestibular and somatosensory systems to generate motor commands, resulting in muscular responses or motor output (Peterka, 2002). During standing, two postural control mechanisms can be used to accelerate the CoM in relation to the base of support. The first of these is activating muscles around the ankle to generate ankle moments (Hof, 2007; Horak & Nashner, 1986). These ankle moments are reflected in a shift of the center of pressure of the ground reaction force (CoP). Consequently, this mechanism has been coined the “CoP mechanism” (Hof, 2007; Horak & Nashner, 1986). The second mechanism is changing the angular momentum around the CoM to change the direction of the ground reaction force, i.e., the “counter-rotation mechanism” (Hof, 2007) . Rotation of the trunk and pelvis around the hip, which has been called the hip mechanism in the literature, is one example of the counter-rotation mechanism (Hof, 2007; Otten, 1999). Other examples of the counter-rotation mechanism are accelerations of other body segments, such as the arms or head, which can be used in the same way. The use of these postural control mechanisms has been suggested to be direction-specific (Winter et al., 1996; Winter et al., 1998). Use of the CoP-mechanism in anteroposterior direction involves modulation of plantar and dorsiflexor muscle activity. Use of the CoP-mechanism in mediolateral direction involves modulation of evertor and invertor muscle activity, but in bipedal stance also involves loading and unloading the legs by extensor and flexor muscle activity respectively. In anteroposterior direction, hip and trunk flexor/extensor muscle activity and in mediolateral direction, hip abductor/adductor and trunk lateroflexor muscle activity are involved in the counter-rotation mechanism to rotate the trunk and consequently accelerate the CoM or the arms. In a previous paper, we focused on the use of the postural control mechanism in the frontal plane (i.e., mediolateral direction) (van den Bogaart et al., 2022). The current manuscript focusses on the use of the postural control mechanisms in the sagittal plane (i.e., anteroposterior direction) in the same population.
During quiet bipedal stance, the ankle mechanism is the dominant mechanism to accelerate the CoM in the sagittal plane in healthy younger adults (Winter et al., 1998). More proximal muscles will be activated when standing on a compliant or moving support surface (Patel et al., 2008; Riemann et al., 2003). Standing on such a surface makes proprioceptive information at the ankle less reliable, as it is not directly related to verticality and changes the effects of ankle moments on CoM acceleration (Horak & Hlavacka, 2001; MacLellan & Patla, 2006). Therefore, it is expected that people will rely more on the counter-rotation mechanism as an addition to the CoP mechanism with increased task difficulty (e.g., surface instability). The frequency of postural corrections made using the CoP mechanism could be increased when standing on a moving support surface as the mean power frequency (MPF) of CoP displacements is higher when standing on a sway-referenced support surface compared to a rigid surface (Dickin et al., 2012).
At both the beginning and the end of the lifespan, challenges with postural control are common. In children, immature sensory systems limit postural control (Steindl et al., 2006). Maturation of the somatosensory system occurs at 3 to 4 years of age and the visual and vestibular systems reach adult levels at 15 to 16 years of age (Steindl et al., 2006) or even later (Hirabayashi & Iwasaki, 1995). The integration and re-weighting of sensory information is not yet adult-like until the age of 15 (Shams et al., 2020). This could explain the differences in balance performance between children and adults. During quiet standing and standing on foam, the amplitude of CoP displacements, CoP velocities and CoM accelerations and the MPF of CoM accelerations, are larger in children between 3 and 6 years old than in older children and adults (Hsu et al., 2009; Oba et al., 2015). In addition to the differences in balance performance, children between 4 and 6 years old showed variable use of postural control mechanisms after disturbances of upright standing by a movable platform. Sometimes the children demonstrated an ankle mechanism, and sometimes they demonstrated a counter-rotation mechanism (specifically the hip mechanism) (Shumway-Cook & Woollacott, 1985). It was postulated that children do not show adult-like consistent use of the postural control mechanisms until 10 years of age (Shumway-Cook & Woollacott, 1985). Information on the use of the CoP mechanism and counter-rotation mechanism when standing on different (unstable) surfaces in children is, to the best of our knowledge, missing.
In older adults, deterioration of the sensory and motor systems, as well as sensory re-weighting deficits occur (L. Sturnieks et al., 2008). Deficits in the sensorimotor control system in older adults lead to impaired balance performance compared to younger adults (Toledo & Barela, 2010). CoM accelerations and MPF of CoP velocities were larger in older adults (age > 70) compared to younger adults during quiet standing (Demura & Kitabayashi, 2006; Masani et al., 2007; Yu et al., 2008). The amplitude of CoP displacements was larger after forward platform translations when comparing older adults (age > 65) with younger adults (Nakamura et al., 2001). When comparing the use of the postural control mechanisms between older and young people, older adults tend to use the counter-rotation mechanism more after perturbations of standing (Gu et al., 1996; Liaw et al., 2009; Lin et al., 2004; Manchester et al., 1989). Information on the use of the CoP mechanism and counter-rotation mechanism in the anteroposterior direction when standing on different (unstable) surfaces in older adults is still missing and worthwhile to assess.
We assessed if, and how, children, younger adults and older adults use the counter-rotation mechanism to accelerate the CoM during standing and how this interacts with the CoP mechanism, during standing on unstable support surfaces, i.e., uniaxial balance boards that can freely move in the sagittal plane. To test if, and how, balance performance and the related use of the postural control mechanisms change with ageing, variations in surface instability were used. We expected poorer balance performance and more use of the counter-rotation mechanism in children and older adults compared to younger adults. We also expected poorer balance performance and increased use of the counter-rotation mechanism during standing on the balance boards compared to standing on a rigid surface. Additionally, we hypothesized that the CoP mechanism is dominant, based on our findings when assessing the use of the postural control mechanisms in the frontal plane (van den Bogaart et al., 2022) .
2 Methods
The methods and participants of this study were identical to that of our previous study (van den Bogaart et al., 2022). In the current experimental setup, however, the direction of the movement of the balance boards was in the anteroposterior direction (i.e., in the sagittal plane). Whereas in our previous study the balance boards only allowed movement in the mediolateral direction (i.e., in the frontal plane).
2.1 Subjects
Sixteen pre-pubertal children between 6-9 years old (10 males, age 8.2±1.1 years old, BMI 15.6±1.5 kg/m2), 17 healthy younger adults between 18-24 years old (7 males, age 21.9±1.6 years old, BMI 23.5±3.0 kg/m2) and eight older adults between 65-80 years old (5 males, age 71.8±4.6 years old, BMI 26.0±3.4 kg/m2) participated. Sample size was calculated for a two-tailed unpaired sample t-test analysis using G*Power (1-\(\beta\) = 0.8, \(\alpha\) = 0.05) and an effect size of 1.5 based on previous studies (Masani et al., 2007; Oba et al., 2015). The required sample size calculated was eight per group (Supplementary Materials A.1.2). Potential participants were excluded if they reported any neurological or orthopedic disorder(s), had an uncorrectable visual impairment, were unable to maintain independent and unsupported stance for 60 seconds, had a BMI > 30 kg/m2, had undergone surgery of the lower extremities during the last two years, or took medication that might affect postural control. Additionally, older subjects were excluded if they had experienced two or more falls during normal daily activities in the preceding year or had a cognitive impairment (tested with Mini-Mental state examination (score<24)). Participants gave written informed consent prior to the experiment. The study protocol was in agreement with the declaration of Helsinki and had been approved by the local ethical committee (CME2018/064, NCT04050774).
2.2 Research design
The participants performed bipedal upright standing on a rigid surface and on three balance boards varying in height of the surface above the point of contact with the floor (BB1; 15 cm, BB2; 17 cm and BB3; 19 cm). The difficulty of the task is affected by the height of the surface of the board above the point of contact with the floor van den Bogaart et al. (2022). The balance board was a 48 cm by 48 cm wooden board mounted on a section of a cylinder with a 24 cm radius that could freely move in the sagittal plane (Figure 1). Participants were allowed to practice standing on BB1 (the balance board with the smallest height) for 16 seconds before the actual measurements started. The four conditions were repeated three times in random order, with each trial lasting 16 seconds. Based on pilot tests prior to the start of the experiments, we found that trials longer than 16 seconds seemed not feasible as these resulted in frequent falling/stepping off the balance board across all age groups. Furthermore, the longer the trial duration, the more aspects like attention and motivation were tested and challenged, which could be confounding factors when assessing balance control, especially in young children. For every trial, participants were instructed to stand barefoot on two feet, placed in parallel at hip width and arms along the body. They were asked to stand as still as possible and look at a marked spot at seven meters distance on the wall in front of them at eye level.
2.3 Materials and Software
A Simi 3D motion analysis system (GmbH) with eight cameras (sample rate: 100 samples/sec, resolution: 1152x864 pixels) and 48 retro reflective markers was used. The illumination in the room at eye level was 650 Lux. Full body 3D kinematics (16 segments) were retrieved using the open-source deep learning python toolboxes DeepLabCut https://github.com/AlexEMG/DeepLabCut) and Anipose https://github.com/lambdaloop/anipose). The complete workflow has been described previously (van den Bogaart et al., 2022).
2.4 Data Analysis
2.4.1 Performance
A trial was registered as a balance loss if a stepping response or an intervention by a researcher was required in order to remain standing. The number of balance losses per condition and per age group was recorded as a performance related outcome measure. In case of balance loss, the trial was excluded from further analysis without redoing the trial. Next to the number of balance losses, the standard deviation (SD) of the time series of the CoM acceleration in the sagittal plane was determined as a measure of performance.
2.4.2 Postural control mechanisms
The magnitudes of CoM acceleration induced by the CoP mechanism and counter-rotation mechanism in the sagittal plane were calculated using Equation 1, as described by Hof (2007).
\[ {\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\left(t\right)=\ \frac{-F_z\ \left({CoP}_{AP}\left(t\right)-{CoM}_{AP}\left(t\right)\right)}{m\ \cdot\ {CoM}_{vertical}\left(t\right)}+\ \frac{{\dot{H}}_{sag}(t)}{m\ \cdot\ {CoM}_{vertical}\left(t\right)} \tag{1}\]
in which m is body mass, CoMAP is the anteroposterior (AP) position of the CoM, CoMvertical is the vertical position of the CoM, \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) is the double derivative of CoMAP with respect to time, t is time, Fz is the vertical ground reaction force, CoPAP is the AP position of the CoP, and Ḣsag is the change in total body angular momentum in the sagittal plane.
Here, the first part of the right-hand term, \(\frac{-F_z\ \left({CoP}_{AP}\left(t\right)-{CoM}_{AP}\left(t\right)\right)}{m\ \cdot\ {CoM}_{vertical}\left(t\right)}\), refers to the CoP mechanism and the second part, \(\frac{{\dot{H}}_{sag}(t)}{m\ \cdot\ {CoM}_{vertical}\left(t\right)}\), is the AP CoM acceleration induced by the counter-rotation mechanism. Due to a technical problem, it was not possible to collect accurate ground reaction forces and CoP, the magnitude of AP CoM acceleration induced by the CoP mechanism was calculated by subtracting, \(\frac{{\dot{H}}_{sag}(t)}{m\ \cdot\ {CoM}_{vertical}\left(t\right)}\) , from \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}(t)\). The SD values of the time series of CoM acceleration due to CoP and counter-rotation mechanism were calculated for each trial. The relative contributions of the CoP and counter-rotation mechanism to the \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) (in %) were calculated by dividing the SD of each mechanism by \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) , multiplied by 100. Totals higher than 100% indicate opposite effects of both mechanisms (Supplementary Materials A.1.3).
2.4.3 Kinematics
Orientations of the board and head in the sagittal plane were calculated relative to the global coordinate system. The deviations from the mean orientations and the MPF of the balance board orientation and \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) were calculated as described previously to provide a better understanding of the use of the CoP and counter-rotation mechanism (van den Bogaart et al., 2022). To determine if participants prioritize to keep their head stable rather than using the upper body rotations as a counter-rotation mechanism to accelerate the CoM, we calculated the deviations from the mean orientations of the board and the head to see if the head rotates along with the balance board. The MPF of the balance board orientation reflects the frequency of CoP shifts and thus partially reflects CoM acceleration due to the CoP mechanism. To determine if this coincides with the frequency of total CoM acceleration, the MPF of the \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was calculated.
The data and code for the analysis can be found at https://osf.io/e6zvx/.
2.5 Statistics
The number of trials for each surface condition was three unless balance loss occurred, which resulted in exclusion of this trial. Fisher exact tests were used to compare the number of balance losses of older adults and children with younger adults. The results of the successful trials of each surface condition were averaged for each participant. Mixed model ANOVAs were used to determine the effect of Age and Surface as well as their interaction on the SD of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\), SD of CoM acceleration due to the CoP and counter-rotation mechanism, the relative contribution of the CoP and counter-rotation mechanism to \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\), the SD of the balance board and head orientation, and the MPF of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) and balance board orientation. In case of a significant main effect, post-hocs on the main effects were performed (using a Bonferroni correction of \(\alpha\)). In case of a significant interaction effect, post-hoc analyses were performed to determine differences between the surface conditions per age group (via repeated measures ANOVAs per age group using a Bonferroni correction of \(\alpha\) = \(\alpha\)/6). In addition, to compare children and older adults with younger adults, post-hoc analyses (via unpaired t-tests per surface condition, using a Bonferroni correction of \(\alpha\) = \(\alpha\)/2) were done to compare children with younger adults and older adults with younger adults. Statistical analyses were performed with SPSS(v25) with \(\alpha\)<0.05.
3 Results
In spite of slight deviations from normality, parametric statistical testing was performed. Transforming data hampers the interpretation of the results and ANOVA is considered robust to violations of normality (Schmider et al., 2010).
3.1 Performance
3.1.1 Balance loss
None of the participants had to be excluded because at least one out of the three trials per surface condition per participant was available. Older adults did lose balance more often than younger adults, 50% versus 5.9% respectively (Table 1, p = 0.023).
RIGID | BB1 | BB2 | BB3 | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Child | 1x | |||
Child | 1x | |||
Child | 1x | 1x | ||
Younger adult | 1x | |||
Older adult | 2x | 2x | 2x | |
Older adult | 1x | 1x | 2x | |
Older adult | 2x | 1x | 1x | |
Older adult | 1x | 2x | 1x |
3.1.2 Total CoM acceleration
Significant main effects of Age, Surface and a significant interaction of Age and Surface on the SD of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) were found (Figure 2 A). The SD of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly larger in children and older adults compared to younger adults across all conditions. In addition, the SD of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly smaller during standing on a rigid surface compared to standing on the balance boards across all age groups. The SD of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly smaller during standing on a rigid surface compared to standing on BB1 in children and younger adults, but not significantly different in older adults (p = 0.052). The SD of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly larger in older adults and children compared to younger adults in the balance board conditions, but no significant difference was found between younger adults and older adults when standing on a rigid surface.
3.2 Postural control mechanisms
3.2.1 CoP mechanism
Significant main effects of Age, Surface and a significant interaction of Age and Surface on the SD of the contribution of the CoP mechanism were found (Figure 2 B). The SD of CoM acceleration due to the CoP mechanism was significantly larger in children and older adults compared to younger adults across all conditions. Furthermore, the SD of CoM acceleration due to the CoP mechanism was significantly smaller during standing on a rigid surface compared to standing on the balance boards across all age groups. The SD of CoM acceleration due to the CoP mechanism was significantly larger in older adults compared to younger adults during standing on BB1 and BB3, but no significant difference was found between younger adults and older adults when standing on a rigid surface and on BB2 (p = 0.05). Moreover, the SD of CoM acceleration due to the CoP mechanism was significantly smaller during standing on a rigid surface compared to standing on BB1 in children and younger adults, but not in older adults (p = 0.056). The relative contribution of the CoP mechanism to \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) ranged from 95%-108%. (Figure 2 D). The average relative contribution decreased from standing on a rigid surface to standing on the balance boards (effect of Surface, Figure 2 D).
3.2.2 Counter-rotation mechanism
Significant main effects of Age, Surface and a significant interaction of Age and Surface on the SD of CoM acceleration due to the counter-rotation mechanism were found (Figure 2 C). The SD of CoM acceleration due to the counter-mechanism was significantly larger in children and older adults compared to younger adults across all conditions. Moreover, the SD of CoM acceleration due to the counter-rotation mechanism was significantly smaller during standing on a rigid surface compared to standing on the balance boards across all groups. In addition, the SD of CoM acceleration due to the counter-rotation mechanism was significantly smaller during standing on BB1 compared to standing on BB3 across all groups. The SD of CoM acceleration due to the counter-mechanism was significantly larger in older adults compared to younger adults during standing on BB1 and BB2, but not when standing on a rigid surface and on BB3. The SD of CoM acceleration due to the counter-rotation mechanism increased significantly with increasing height of the balance board in children and younger adults, with differences between standing on BB1 and BB3, but did not significantly increase in older adults. The relative contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism to \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) ranged from 19%-31%. (Figure 2 E). The relative contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism was significantly larger in children compared to younger adults (effect of Age, Figure 2 E), but was not different between older and younger adults. Moreover, the relative contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism to \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) increased with surface instability, with differences between standing on a rigid surface and BB1 on one hand and BB3 on the other hand, and between standing on BB1 and BB2 (Effect of Surface, Figure 2 E).
3.3 Kinematics
3.3.1 Balance board orientation
The SD of balance board orientation was larger in older adults compared to younger adults (effect of Age, Figure 3 A). The SD of balance board orientation was significantly smaller when standing on BB1 than when standing on BB3 (effect of Surface, Figure 3 A).
3.3.2 Head orientation
A significant main effect of Age and a significant interaction of Age and Surface on the SD of head orientation were found (Figure 3). The SD of head rotation was significantly larger in children compared to younger adults across all conditions. Post-hoc tests did not reveal significant effects.
3.3.3 MPF of CoM accelerations and balance board rotations
Significant main effects of Age, Surface and a significant interaction of Age and Surface on the MPF of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) were found (Figure 3 C). The MPF of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly larger in older adults compared to younger adults across all conditions. Moreover, the MPF of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly smaller during standing on a rigid surface compared to standing on the balance boards across all groups. The MPF of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly larger in older adults compared to younger adults during standing on BB1 and BB2 (BB3; p = 0.06). In older adults, the MPF of \({\ddot{CoM}}_{AP}\) was significantly lower during standing on the rigid surface compared to standing on BB3, but not so in younger adults and children. The MPF of balance board orientation was significantly larger in children and older adults compared to younger adults (effect of Age, Figure 3 D). Moreover, the MPF of balance board orientation increased with surface instability, with differences between standing on BB1 and BB2 on one hand and standing on BB3 on the other hand (effect of Surface) (BB1 versus BB2; p = 0.057).
4 Discussion
We assessed if, and how, children, younger adults and older adults use the counter-rotation mechanism to accelerate their CoM during standing and how this interacts with the CoP mechanism, during standing on moving support surfaces, i.e., uniaxial balance boards that could freely move in the sagittal plane. As hypothesized, we found poorer balance performance in children and older adults compared to younger adults. Across age groups and conditions, the contribution of the CoP mechanism to the total CoM acceleration was dominant. The contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism was much smaller. Contrary to our hypothesis, only children and not the older adults did use the counter-rotation mechanism more to accelerate the CoM than younger adults.
4.1 Effects of surface instability
A total of 23 balance losses occurred spread over trials from three children, one younger adult and four older adults, all when standing on a balance board. The SD of CoM accelerations was larger during standing on a balance board than on the rigid surface, reflecting that standing on the balance board was indeed more challenging than standing on the floor. Decreased pertinence of proprioceptive information from ankle muscles and a reduction of the effectiveness of ankle moments to accelerate the CoM when standing on a balance board could be an explanation for this (Horak & Hlavacka, 2001; van Dieën et al., 2015). The number of balance losses (23), with balance boards that could freely move in the sagittal plane, was much larger than in our previous study, with uniaxial balance boards that could freely move in the frontal plane (only 3) (van den Bogaart et al., 2022). Surface instability in the sagittal plane thus seems more challenging compared to surface instability in the frontal plane. This could be due to the fact that establishing CoP shifts by loading and unloading the legs by extensor and flexor muscle activity respectively, is possible in the frontal plane (D. Winter et al., 1993), next to applying ankle moments. Increasing the height of the balance boards did lead to larger deviations from the mean balance board orientation, increased frequency of balance board rotations and increased SD values and relative contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism. In contrast, no effects of balance board height were found on balance boards that could freely move in the frontal plane (van den Bogaart et al., 2022).
4.2 Age effects
4.2.1 Performance and kinematics
Despite being healthy and non-falling, the older participants lost balance more often than the younger adult participants, which could indicate a higher risk of falls in daily life. Sensorimotor control is worse in children and older adults compared to younger adults, hence an increased SD of CoM accelerations, corresponding to a deterioration of balance performance, and larger deviations from the mean board orientation compared to younger adults were expected (Bugnariu & Fung, 2006; Hirabayashi & Iwasaki, 1995; Shams et al., 2020; Steindl et al., 2006; Teasdale et al., 1991). The differences in postural control between older adults and children on one hand and younger adults on the other hand during standing on unstable surfaces are in line with previous studies (Bergamin et al., 2014; Hsu et al., 2009; Riach & Starkes, 1989; Sturnieks et al., 2011). However, it could be assumed that these studies assessed ‘steady-state’ postural control (according to Reed et al. (2020)). The postural control during the familiarization period of a new task (i.e., standing on a balance board) was measured in the current study. The age effects in the current study could be a result of differences in time needed to familiarize. The age effects could be different when comparing the steady-state postural control between age groups. The age effects could be different when comparing steady-state postural control between age groups, but since fast learning effects may occur (van Dieën et al., 2015), and may be different between age groups, it is questionable if and how a steady-state can be defined. Despite the instruction to look at a marked spot on the wall in front of them at eye level, children did rotate their head more compared to younger adults. More head rotation in children could be self-generated and indicate less attention, which is common in children compared to younger adults (Huang-Pollock et al., 2002; Wickens, 1974). Self-generated head rotation may be disadvantageous, as it leads to changing visual and vestibular inputs, requiring more processing to discern between body motion and self-imposed head motion (Khan & Chang, 2013). Furthermore, self-generated head rotation could potentially result in increased muscle tone (e.g., leg and arm muscles) due to the tonic neck reflex (Bruijn et al., 2013; Iles & Pisini, 1992; Parr et al., 2008). However, the head rotation in children was only around three degrees. The higher frequency of balance board rotations in children and older adults could potentially reflect an increased frequency of CoM accelerations due to the CoP mechanism, and did coincide with an increased frequency of total CoM accelerations in older adults. However, increased frequency of the CoM accelerations did not lead to improved balance performance. In children, the higher frequency of balance board rotations did not automatically result in an increased frequency of total CoM accelerations as the frequency of CoM corrections depends on the control actions of both the CoP mechanism and the counter-rotation mechanism. It should be kept in mind that board rotations can reflect corrective actions as well as perturbations due to neuromuscular noise. Furthermore, the magnitude of CoM acceleration can indicate control of the CoM relative to the BoS, but perturbation effects on the CoM accelerations cannot be distinguished from control actions.
4.2.2 Postural control mechanisms
The contribution of the CoP mechanism to CoM acceleration was dominant relative to the contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism. The relative contribution of the CoP mechanism to the total CoM acceleration was around 100% (ranging from 95%-108%) and the relative contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism was around 25% (ranging from 19%-31%). The contribution of the two mechanisms was not always in the same direction, as the summed SD values were often larger than the SD values of the total anteroposterior CoM acceleration. However, the desired direction for either of these mechanisms is unclear. We found that children used the counter-rotation mechanism relatively more to accelerate the CoM compared to younger adults. This is in contrast to our previous study using balance boards that could freely move in the frontal plane, in which we did not find an effect of age on the relative use of the counter-rotation mechanism (van den Bogaart et al., 2022). The increased contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism in children cannot be explained by differences in body height between children and younger adults, as accelerating the body center of mass by the counter-rotation mechanism is less efficient at lower height (A.1.1. Supplementary materials, https://osf.io/e6zvx/). The increased amount of head rotation in children is unlikely to have contributed substantially to the increased rate of change of angular momentum (i.e., use of the counter-rotation mechanism) as head rotation was limited to only three degrees. We suggest that children are still learning to limit the contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism to the same extent as younger and older adults (Shumway-Cook & Woollacott, 1985). Overall, the contribution of the counter-rotation mechanism was limited, also in children. It could be that segmental rotations were used to achieve a proper orientation of segments such as regulating the orientation of the head in space, rather than accelerating the CoM (Alizadehsaravi et al., 2021). All participants, even the children, kept their head quite stable. This suggests that people prefer to maintain a constant visual and vestibular input by keeping the head stable, rather than using upper body rotations as a counter-rotation mechanism to accelerate the CoM. In addition, rotational accelerations of body parts need to be reversed leading to the opposite effect on the acceleration of the CoM. We also found limited use of the counter-rotation mechanism to accelerate the CoM in gait, as using the counter-rotation mechanism would actually interfere with the gait pattern (van den Bogaart et al., 2020). During unipedal stance on a balance board, larger contributions of counter-rotation were found, but this was to a large extent generated by the free leg (van Dieën et al., 2015). A limitation of this study is that the study could be underpowered as the sample size calculation was based on t-tests while a mixed model analysis was used. Another limitation of this study is that we assume that the counter-rotation mechanism can be used without changing the position of the CoP, but that in practice, this may not always be the case as this requires precise coordination. The two mechanisms can be distinguished analytically, but whether they are used independently remains to be proven.
4.3 Implementation
Understanding the mechanisms used for postural control could be used to determine training targets. For example, relying more on the counter-rotation mechanism may result in a fall if the angular accelerations cannot be reversed due to (anatomical) constraints (e.g., range of motion, strength, flexibility, reaction time). Moreover, it could cause interference with other task constraints, such as orienting the head in space. Training the use of specific mechanisms to accelerate the CoM could be implemented in therapeutic interventions that aim to improve balance performance (e.g., decreasing fall incidence or decreasing the number of recovery steps after a perturbation). However, whether and how, a specific mechanism can be trained (in specific populations and situations) needs further investigation.
4.4 Conclusions
Children and older adults had a poorer balance performance, than younger adults. Across age groups and conditions, the contribution of the CoP mechanism to the total CoM acceleration was much larger than that of the counter-rotation mechanism. The CoP mechanism was dominant. Increasing the height of the balance board provoked increased use of the counter-rotation mechanism. Furthermore, children used, but older adults did not use, the counter-rotation mechanism relatively more compared to younger adults.
5 Additional Information
5.1 Data Accessibility
The supplementary materials, data and code for the analysis can be found at https://osf.io/e6zvx/
5.3 Conflict of Interest
None. The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.
5.4 Funding
Sjoerd M. Bruijn was supported by grants from the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (016.Vidi.178.014).
5.5 Acknowledgments
The authors gratefully acknowledge The Expertise centre for Digital Media (EDM) for technical support and Kimmy Daenen and Charlotte Uwents for their help during the experiments.